Home

Schizophrenia and Attention: Cognitive and Neurobiological Aspects

This  paper on schizophrenia was written during a postgraduate course in applied psychology and is more a collection of information that I have not had time to edit and refine, but the various references may be of use to some researchers. The thrust of this article is to explain that the while there are 'abnormalities' in cognition and attention in schizophrenia  the underlying  neurobiological differences are not necessarily pathological or inferior. Instead many problems occur because  the resultant differences in cognitive skills and information processing create friction between a small minority of the population and the vast majority including many clinicians. This failure to consider individual differences leads to treatment failures and behavioural psychopathology which results from either extreme arousal levels maintaining frontal-temporal control of information processing, even when hippocampal transfer is impaired resulting in delusions and hallucinations or deliberate refusal to use frontal processing as a self treatment option which results in passivity and faulty reasoning.  

 

 

SCHIZOPHRENIA AND ATTENTION:

COGNITIVE AND NEUROBIOLOGICAL ASPECTS

 

CONTENTS                  

 

1. Background

    Symptoms

    Neurobiological Correlates of Schizophrenia

2. Cognitive Abnormalities

    Memory

    Attention

             Selective

            Sustained

    Summary

3. Neurobiology of Attention

4. Conclusion

5. References

 


 

 

BACKGROUND

 

                    

Memory

 

    1. Free recall - From two sets of 30 words one set was randomly assigned to subjects and they were required to read and then recall as many as possible.
    2. Repetition Priming – 3 letter stems from both sets of 30 words were presented and then scores from the new list were subtracted from scores on the old list to give a score for priming effect.
    3. Procedural Cognitive Skill Learning - This was accomplished with a version of the Tower of Hanoi test called the Tower of Toronto in which subjects are asked to move 4 disks from the left of three pegs to the right most peg. Each disk is lighter or darker than others and arranged in order of darkness with the lightest on top. They must be moved so that a darker disk is never on top of a lighter one. This requires a minimum of 15 moves. SS’s were allowed a maximum of 50 attempts and two sets of 5 trials 90 minutes apart were conducted.
    4. Frequency monitoring – The Rey Auditory verbal learning test was used and SS’s were asked to listen to a list of fifteen words with five of them repeated ie a list of twenty words and then SS’s were asked whether the words were heard once or twice.

This provided two tests of implicit learning (2&3) and two of explicit learning(1&4).

They found that schizophrenics had significant deficits for explicit learning tasks but not for implicit learning tasks when comparisons between schizophrenic patients and normal controls were made. The schizophrenic group also had poorer performance for the Tower of Toronto task but their learning rate over the ten trials was the same as for the control group. This was interpreted as a deficit in problem solving ability, rather than memory or learning capacity, and the fact that explicit memory was affected but implicit memory was unaffected was thought to result from different neurobiological locations for these memories.(they note that tasks which require effort are affected but not automatic processing tasks) . This tends to support the notion that processing capacity may be diminished in schizophrenia but underlying memory systems remain intact.

Gold et al(1992) argued that if attentional capacity is reduced it would be expected that only explicit memory would be affected and the relatively automatic processes involved in implicit memory processes such as recognition memory would be unaffected. This may be the case although recognition memory may also be classified as a form of explicit memory. A further investigation of this will be discussed below. Gold et al(1992) conducted an experiment using recall, recognition and frequency monitoring paradigms, similar to those outlined above, and found that complete memory function, explicit and implicit, was affected. This also included the rate of learning. This study involved 35 schizophrenia patients and 18 control subjects. The patients were all on antipsychotic medication with the exception of one. Because all memory systems were affected similarly they argued that the memory deficits were not caused by a processing capacity deficit. If this had been the case the low capacity required for recognition memory would not have affected this type of memory to the same extent as recall memory. What was not explained was the fact that 22 of the 35 patients were using anticholinergic medication. Acetylcholine is a neurotransmitter that plays a pivotal role in memory processes and it would be expected that even normal subjects taking anticholinergic medication would show memory deficits. Enhancing acetylcholine action by injecting cholinergic agonists into the hippocampus of schizophrenics. It has been shown to reduce the sensory gating deficit, marked by the P50 wave for auditory startle response abnormalities found in schizophrenia. This is an important experimental confound that may well explain the difference between the Gras-Vincendon et al study and the Gold et al study.

Another study (La Porte, Kirkpatrick and Thakar, 1994) found that verbal recall was the same for control subjects with schizotypal personality disorder. They tested whether free recall deficits(verbal and non verbal memory) found in schizophrenic patients (Calev et al, 1983, Calev,1984 and Sengel et al, 1984) were also found in schizotypal personalities.

They used three scales from the Chapman scales originally used to assess schizotypy but now recognised as an assessment for proneness to psychosis.

Perceptual Aberration Scale Social Anhedonia Scale Magical Ideation Scale
eg ‘I have sometimes felt confused as to whether my body was my own.’ eg ‘I prefer hobbies and leisure activities where I am on my own.’ eg ‘At times I have felt that a lecture was meant particularly for me.

Subjects were then tested for verbal memory using two short paragraphs   psychiatric first degree relatives in comparison with nofrom theWeschler Memory scale. This has been assessed as the most sensitive test of verbal memory differences between acute schizophrenic patients and normal controls. Subjects were asked to make immediate recall then tested for recall again after 30 minutes. They found no correlation between recall and proneness to psychosis in either the immediate or the delayed recall condition and when the scores for depession and IQ, which were also scored along with ethnicity, were partialled out, it was found that there was a slight positive correlation with proneness to psychosis and better verbal memory. This was even more pronounced in African-American subjects. This tends to indicate that the verbal recall, ie explicit memory, deficits noted in schizophrenia( Neuchterlein and Dawson, 1984) are related to the disease or medication rather than an innate genetic deficit. Another study that used comprehensive statistical and subject assessment methodologies did however detect verbal memory deficits in both schizophrenics and nonrmals(Cannon et al, 1994).

Given that the neurobiological model for memory places great emphasis on longterm potentiation in the hippocampus, a preferred site for supposed morphological abnormalities in schizophrenia, and that,

it would be expected that storage of ‘data’ would be increased in unmedicated schizophrenics and this is consistent with subjective reports of information overload and inability to sort context from the detail. This would indicate that it is the inaccessibility of data and or the inability to encode in an easily retrievable format which is likely to be the basis of any memory deficit. In terms of motivation it may well be seen as a metamemory deficit in that a refusal to learn an effective strategy for recall processes, which are less important to everyday functioning as opposed to academic or intellectual work, results from a desire to minimise the effort that is required for explicit memory and retrieval. As a result schizophrenics tend to rely on recognition and implicit learning processes. This would be more consistent with the peculiarities of thought and ‘magical’ thinking that is often found in schizophrenics and schizotypal personality eg ‘mystical’ intuitive explanation for common occurrences and events.

In summary it appears that implicit memory processes remain unaffected by schizophrenia and that in some cases explicit memory deficits are found but this appears to be more task dependent and may well be a pathological rather than a predisposing factor.

 

Attention

 

The study of attention in schizophrenia has produced findings which do tend to be regularly replicated among first degree relatives and those with schizotypal personality disorders and this suggests that there is an underlying attentional abnormality not only in those with pathological schizophrenia but also those with a genetic predisposition and that these attentional differences may well be useful as markers for individuals at risk for the disease. The study of attention in schizophrenia has focussed on two areas:

 

Selective Attention

 

The concept of selective attention and its dependence on the processing capacity, as a result of a combination of attention and arousal as outlined in Kahneman’s 1973 attentional model (see figure 4).

Figure4.jpg (14215 bytes)

This is a model that is thought to have a high degree of relevance in explaining selective attentional deficits in schizophrenia(Neuchterlein,1984). Conscious processing is a function of processing capacity but automatic processing is not. The amount of capacity is affected by the level of arousal and the physiological correlates of arousal such as skin conductance response(SCR) is found to be abnormal in those suffering from schizophrenia. This is another indicator that attentional functioning is disordered in schizophrenia(Wieselgren, 1994). The study of selective attention uses approaches such as simple reaction time, choice reaction time, modality shift reaction time, dichotic listening experiments and the Stroop Colour word tests. Variations of these such as reaction time tasks with regular preparatory intervals and irregular preparatory intervals show a crossover effect which usually marks schizophrenia sufferers as while their overall reaction is slower they have an unusual pattern of response in that they, unlike normals, are initially faster with regular preparatory intervals but this crosses over and they become faster with irregular preparatory intervals unlike normals(see figure 5).

1. Schwartz et al(1991) used a simple reaction time test as part of an experiment to assess the relation between negative symptoms and attention. Subjects were asked to press a telegraph key when a tone was heard and at varying periods of 2.5, 3.0 or 3.5 secs a light would be shown and then subjects had to remove their finger. Reaction time was recorded for each movement and then correlations calculated with a negative symptom score taken from the Andreasen scale. The results indicated no relation between positive symptoms and RT but a significant positive relation between negative symptoms and simple reaction time.

Figure9.jpg (25839 bytes)

 

                                                        ©Philip Pocock 1998